Journal of Scientific Exploration, Vol. 24, No. 4, pp. 599-610, 2010

0892-3310/10

0892-3310/10

 

 

Anticipatory Alarm Behaviour in Bengalese finches

FERNANDO ALVAREZ

Estación Biológica de Doñana, CSIC, Departamento de Etología y Conservación de
Biodiversidad, Av. Américo Vespucio s/n, E-41092 Sevilla, Spain
Address for reprints: Fernando Alvarez, Estación Biológica de Doñana, CSIC, CSIC
Av. Américo Vespucio s/n, Isla de La Cartuja, E-41092 Sevilla, Spain

Abstract-The ability for short-term alarm precognition was explored in Bengalese finches. During the experimental trials, subjects were put individu- ally for 20 minutes in a testing cage and in the last 5 minutes a 15-second video clip of a slowly crawling snake was shown to them in a TFT screen. The video clip was presented at random starting out from 20 possible random-ly predetermined options. During the control trials, no snake video clip was shown to the birds. Subjects were filmed, and, in a double-blind fashion, the frequency of their display of alarm was registered 0-3, 3-6, and 6-9 seconds immediately before stimulus presentation and before the same moment of the control trial for each bird. As a second control, behavior frequency was also registered immediately before the 10-minute point after the initiation of the experimental trial. The results showed that the birds reacted to the snake video clip at least 9 seconds before presentation, the frequency of the alarm display during that period being higher than that during both controls. Females and males did not differ significantly in any of the measures.

Keywords: Subtle Keywords: alarm display-Bengalese finches-birds-precognition

 

Introduction

There has been an upsurge of interest in human, unconscious, short-term anticipatory response, or precognition, especially since the first paper by Dean Radin on electrodermal activity and presentiments (Radin, 1997a).

Anomalous anticipatory effects using galvanic skin response as a conditioned-dependent variable were first explored with success by Vassy in the late 1960s, but reported later (Vassy, 1978). In this study, all data were obtained by human observation. In later and more exact investigations, electrodermal activity (Skin Conductance Level (SCL), which measures fast and slower-moving changes), was the unconscious response variable in experiments in which a series of randomly selected photographs(so that the incoming stimuli could not be inferred) was shown to the subjects. SCL increased more if the future picture (next 2-3 seconds) was going to be emotional (shocking photos, such as erotic scenes and autopsies), than if it was going to be a calm one (pleasant and neutral photos of nature, landscapes, and cheerful people). Anticipation was also manifested, although at lower intensity, by an increase in heart rate and a reduction in fingertip blood volume (Radin, 1997a, 2004). Electrodermal activity was also significantly higher before presentation of the emotional pictures (as compared to that before the calm pictures) when Skin Conductance Response (SCR, which measures only fast-moving changes in electrodermal activity) was the responding variable (Bierman & Radin, 1997, 1999). The fact that participants anticipate much better the presentation of positive emotional pictures (generally erotic) than negative emotional pictures (violent and injurious) (Radin, 1997b) suggests that the emotional meaning of the future event may be important.

Another study (Globish,Hamm, Esteves, & ohman, 1999) concerned with skin conductance took advantage of a previously published conventional study (i.e. with completely different goals) about the speed with which fear arises in animal-phobic subjects, and with a long enough period of measurement before the stimulus. Re-analysis of the raw data searching for a precognition effect (Bierman, 2000) showed a larger anticipation to occur before the erotic stimuli as compared to neutral (marginally significant) and animal (snakes or spiders) (non significant) stimuli.

To avoid the problem of idiosyncratic responses to pictorial stimuli, random audio startle stimuli and control moments of silence have been used. In this case, the effect of anticipation was revealed by a higher increase in relative skin conductance before the stimulus than before the control (Spottiswoode & May, 2003, May, Paulinyi, & Vassy, 2005).

Heart Rate (HR) was registered while subjects were asked to choose one out of four calm (non-emotional) pictures, the randomly selected picture being selected before they made their choice or after making their choice but not knowing about it. The experiment yielded very significant changes in HR before correct choices as compared to failed ones in the first situation, and still signifi- cant in the second experimental situation (Sartori, Massaccesi, Martinelli, & Tressoldi, 2004, Tressoldi, Martinelli, Massaccesi, & Sartori, 2005). The use of Heart Rate Variability (HRV, which refers to beat-to-beat alterations in HR, and is usually regarded as an indicator of cardiovascular autonomic control) during presentation of calm and emotionally arousing pictures produced still higher effects. This experiment resulted in a significantly higher heart rate deceleration occurring prior to future emotional stimuli compared to calm stimuli, starting about 4.75 seconds before stimulus presentation and showing some gender differences. Comparison of these results with those in the medical literature in relation to cardiac decelerations/accelerations suggests that the psychophysiologic system processes the prestimulus information in the same way as conventional sensory input (McCraty, Atkinson, & Bradley, 2004a).

Several important results were also obtained in a similar experimental design where data were also collected from the electrocardiogram (ECG) and the electroencephalogram (EEG). Information was obtained from the latter on cortical Event-Related Potentials (ERP, the changes of the ongoing EEG due to stimulation, especially clear as a response to unpredictable stimuli), Heartbeat- Evoked brain Potentials (HEP, in which characteristic brain waves are seen to accompany rhythmic activity of the heart), and cardiac decelerations/accel- erations (derived from the ECG). The results showed that: 1) There were sig- nificant differences in prestimulus ERPs and HEPs for calm versus emotional stimuli. 2) Both the heart and the brain are involved in stimulus anticipation, the heart apparently receiving and responding to prestimulus information before the brain. 3) The frontal cortex, temporal, occipital, and parietal areas appear to be involved in the process. 4) An interaction between HEPs and ERPs is espe- cially apparent in females (McCraty, Atkinson, & Bradley, 2004b).

The use of functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging system (fMRI) to obtain information on Blood Oxygenation Level Dependence (BOLD, mag- netic signal variation related to oxygen intake and neuron activity) enables the observation of fast-changing events in the brain. This technique was used while the participants were presented erotic, violent, and calm images, and showed that the above-quoted anomalous anticipatory effects are replicated with BOLD: Anticipatory activation of the visual cortex preceding emotional stimuli was larger than the anticipatory activation preceding neutral stimuli (before the erotic stimuli in males and before the erotic and violent stimuli in females) (Bierman & Scholte, 2002).

Although there is no reason to suppose that humans are better-equipped than other animals with respect to their ability to foresee the future, the litera- ture on precognition by non-humans is very scant. In any case, the results of the few studies are quite promising, and if the effect is definitely demonstrated in them, the biological mechanisms involved in precognition would be open to research.

In an experiment performed with earthworms, in which the response (small changes in skin impedance) to mechanical vibration was recorded, the nearly significant results were more positive the more trials were performed, and the time window in this case would extend one second before stimulation (Wildey, 2001).

The results of experiments with dogs can be interpreted as evidence for precognition. In these studies the animals showed an anticipatory behavior long before their owners returned home, the owners returning at randomly selected times and travelling in unfamiliar vehicles (Sheldrake & Smart, 1998, 2000). Analysis of environmental variables suggests that the dogs' anticipatory behav- ior is significantly affected by changes in global geomagnetic flux (Radin, 2002).

Therefore, the cumulative evidence strongly suggests that short-term pre- cognition occurs in humans and other animals. The subjects apparently can respond physiologically to an emotionally arousing stimulus seconds before it is actually experienced, as if the body's perceptual apparatus were continually scanning the future.

Since predation is a major selective force in the evolution of morphological, physiological, and behavioral characteristics of animals, the alarm response to predation is probably a good candidate among different activities when trying to detect the capacity for an anticipatory response in them. In addition, the behavior of alarm is unfailingly produced by naive animals in the presence of danger, and is sufficiently conspicuous for reliable data collection (Alvarez, Braza, & Norzagaray, 1976, Alvarez, Sanchez, & Angulo, 2006, Alvarez, 1993, Alvarez & Sanchez, 2003). The capacity for anticipatory perception of alarm as well as potential gender differences will be explored in Bengalese finches.

 

Methods

All subjects (23 adult male and 24 adult female Bengalese finches Lonchura striata var. domestica) lived in unisexual adult groups of conspecifics in 1.5 x 3 x 2 m aviaria near Seville, Spain, and were fed a varied diet of seeds and vegetables. The experiments were carried out between December 1, 2008, and April 30, 2009. Sex of Bengalese finches was determined from blood samples (Griffiths, Double, Orr, & Dawson, 1998).

Estrildine finches, among them the Bengalese finch, are potentially good subjects for this kind of study, since they are easily maintained in captivity (Vriends & Heming-Vriends, 2002) and show a very distinct behavior of alarm in the presence of potential predators (mainly tail-flicking, wing-flapping, and alarm calls) (Goodwin, 1982, Lombardi & Curio, 1985a, 1985b, Zann, 1996, Marler, 2004).

Starting between 7:30 and 10:30 UT, the subjects (one at a time) were taken from their groups and put in the 70 x 35 x 35 cm testing cage with transparent glass at both ends (figure 1), where they were out of sight from other birds. Through a wide-open window the subjects could see the area with vegetation outside the aviarium, since being visually enclosed apparently reduces their expression of alarm (Lombardi & Curio, 1985a). During the experimental trials, after a period of time of 15 minutes, a video clip of 15 seconds duration of a slowly crawling horseshoe whip snake Coluber hippocrepis was shown in a TFT screen located at one end of the testing cage. The video clip was shown at random by a true random number generator (Orion Electronics), starting with


one of 20 possible options (unknown to the observer during the tests, and also randomly determined, Table 1) in the five minutes following the 15-minute period. The computer was located in an adjacent room and was connected to the TFT screen by a long cable.



Table1

Although still images have been used to elicit behavior in the Bengalese finch (Watanabe & Jian, 1993), the use of video playback is usually more effective in birds (Dawkins & Woodington, 1997, D'Eath, 1998, Fleishman, McClintock, D'Eath, Brainard, & Endler, 1998, Cuthill, Hart, Partridge, Bennett, Hunt, & Church, 2000). Then, while cathode ray tube (CRT) screens are not appropriate to present stimuli to songbirds, thin film transistor (TFT) screens have been used with great success to obtain responses toward presented video clips from Bengalese and zebra finches (Ikebuchi & Okanoya, 1999, Galloch & Bischof, 2006, 2007).

During the control trial (separated 33-49 days from the experimental trial, their order being determined at random by using the true random number gen- erator), only the same video clip background used during the experimental trials was shown to the same birds, also with a duration of 20 minutes. For 26 subjects the sequence was experimental control, and for 21 subjects control experimental.

During the whole experimental and control trial, each subject was filmed with a 25-frames-per-second video camera (a Sony DCR-SR72E), located out- side the testing cage at the opposite end of the TFT screen (figure 1), and con- trolled by the experimenter from a hidden location in an adjacent room.

Video analysis allows quantification of the intensity of alarm, which in the Bengalese finch is especially conspicuous. The alarm display lasts 0.1-0.2 sec- onds and consists first of very rapid flapping of both wings and then an abrupt sideways thrust of the whole body, and especially of the tail (figure 2). The frequency of this behavior in 3-, 6-, and 9-second blocks immediately before the presentation of the snake stimulus was compared to that immediately before the same moment of the trials when no snake stimulus was shown, but only the same video clip background (control 1). As an additional control (control 2), the pre-stimulation frequency was compared to that occurring immediately before the 10-minute time point after the initiation of the same trial (when only the video clip background was shown). During frame-by-frame film analysis (accuracy of 0.04 seconds), I counted the frequency per 3-, 6-, and 9-second blocks in the above-indicated periods of the subjects' alarm behavior, using the VideoDubMod 1.5.10.2 computer program. To prevent subjective bias, 9-second video clips segments immediately before the stimulus and during the two controls were extracted from the recordings by another person, and the key that identified the segments was kept by a third party (the computer department of the institute). Once those video clip segments were analyzed by the author, without knowing whether each segment would correspond to a particular sub- ject or to the stimulus or the controls, that key was provided.

Statistical Analysis

When separate analyses were done for males and females, nonparametric tests (Friedman ANOVA, Wilcoxon matched pairs test, Mann-Whitney U test, Siegel & Castellan, 1988, Zar, 1996) were used. When applying parametric tests, a log10(x+1) transformation of behavior frequencies was carried out, and the resulting distributions did not depart from normality (p = 0.10-0.20, Kolmogorov-Smirnov test). The data were analysed using a repeated-measures ANOVA, in which control 1, control 2, and pre-stimulus measurements of the same experimental bird composed the replicate data. The t-test for dependent samples was used to compare pairs of distributions (Sokal & Rohlf, 1995, Zar, 1996). Data analysis was done using the STATISTICA 6.0 computer program. All reported p are two-tailed.

Results

When the frequencies of the behavior of alarm for the three periods of 0-3, 3-6, and 6-9 seconds before stimulus presentation were compared, they were found to belong to the same population for both females (c2 = 0.24, N = 24, df = 2, p = 0.886; Friedman ANOVA) and males (c2 = 1.73, N = 23, df = 2, p = 0.421). Consequently, I lumped together the three periods and considered for the rest of the analysis for males and females the 0-9 seconds pre-stimulus period.

Comparisons of behavior frequencies among the pre-stimulus period and the two control periods yielded significant results for both sexes (females: c2 = 9.06, N = 24, df = 2, p < 0.018; males: c2 = 12.31, N = 23, df = 2, p < 0.0012; Friedman ANOVA). Post-hoc two-variable comparisons showed, also for both sexes, that behavior frequencies for the two control periods were statistically similar (females: N = 24, T = 106, p = 0.682; males: N = 23, T = 112.5, p = 0.650; Wilcoxon matched pairs test). Furthermore, and also for both sexes, the pre-stimulus period showed significantly higher behavior frequencies than for control 1 (females: N = 24, T = 62, p < 0.036; males: N = 23, T = 29.5, p < 0.008) and control 2 (females: N = 24, T = 27.5, p < 0.002; males: N = 23, T = 35, p < 0.005).

The ratio of the frequency of the alarm display during the 9 seconds preceding stimulation to 1 plus the average frequency during the two 9-second controls was considered an adequate index of individual precognition ability (IPA). When the values for the two sexes were compared, no significant difference was obtained (IPA: mean + SE; females: 1.59 + 0.26, N = 24; males: 1.77 + 0.27, N = 23; U = 247, p = 0.537, Mann-Whitney U test).

The frequencies of the alarm display for females and males in the 9-second blocks during the experimental and control trials are presented in Table 2.

The absence of male-female significance differences enables the analysis of combined information for both sexes. In this case, the normalized pre-

stimulation values during the three periods of 0–3, 3–6, and 6–9 seconds were also found to belong to the same population (F2, 92 = 0.60, p = 0.552; repeatedmeasures ANOVA). Consequently the 0–9 seconds pre-stimulus period was considered for the rest of this study.

Joint analysis of the values for the pre-stimulation and the two controls periods yielded a highly significant result (F2, 92 = 10.12, p < 0.001). Post-hoc two-variable comparisons showed the values for the pre-stimulation period to be significantly higher than those for control 1 and control 2 (t = 3.56, P < 0.001, t = 4.49, p < 0.001, respectively; df = 46; t-test for dependent samples). Values for both controls were not significantly different (t = 0.93, df = 46, p = 0. 357) (see Figure 3).

Discussion

The higher frequency of the alarm display immediately before stimulation, as compared to that during the two controls, supports the idea that Bengalese finches may be able to anticipate short-term future events, also suggesting that transtemporal perception is not limited to humans. Then, and contrary to findings in humans (Bierman & Scholte, 2002, McCraty, Atkinson, & Bradley, 2004b), no sex difference was found in Bengalese finches

If human unconscious precognition is actually an old evolutionary mechanism in which the brain and the autonomic nervous system play a role (Bierman & Scholte, 2002, McCraty, Atkinson, & Bradley, 2004a, 2004b, Sartori, Massaccesi, Martinelli, & Tressoldi, 2004, Tressoldi, Martinelli, Massaccesi, & Sartori, 2005), and this ability has also been detected in other animals (Sheldrake & Smart, 1998, 2000, Wildey, 2001, present study), we may ask ourselves whether the different nervous systems may perform more ample functions than the classical elaboration of the information received through the ordinary sense organs (Laszlo, 2009).

Although the existence of precognition has been settled a long time back (Rhine, Smith, & Woodruff, 1938, Schmidt, 1969), the question still remains as to how it works. On the one hand, no explanation has been found so far in the deterministic framework of classical physics, as precognition seems to violate the causality principle. On the other hand, it does not fit in the unpredictable scheme of quantum physics either, suggesting that quantum theory, when applied to humans and other living systems, may not always be correct (Schmidt, 1993).

According to Bierman’s (2008) psychophysical theory of Consciousness Induced Restoration of Time-Symmetry (CIRTS), the consciousness-sustaining brain can partially restore the break in time-symmetry, thus allowing for time-reversed processes like precognition. In a broad sense, the data for these Bengalese finches accommodate that hypothesis

The ability to anticipate the presence of a predator by the Bengalese finches and other prey species would obviously increase their chances of surviving. If there would be a definite relationship between the precognitive response and the intensity or quality of the emotional event, and since the latter are expected to differ between both parties (potential predator and prey), the ongoing evolutionary arms race (Vermeij, 1987) would be asymmetrical, and perhaps to the advantage of the potential prey.

Several areas of research are more open when subjects are animals, such as selective breeding for enhanced hereditary precognition ability, exploration of physiological mechanisms involved in precognition, relationship with sex and age classes, and with social organization and personality profiles.

Although most psi studies need to be done in the controlled conditions of the laboratory, observation and experiment of animals in natural conditions should not be discarded, as they could provide hints about the natural selection and adaptive value of short-term alarm precognition.

Acknowledgments

I thank Manuel Vázquez Castro for help in finch maintenance, Enrique Collado for preparing the computer program for random stimuli presentation, and Dick J. Bierman and Dean I. Radin for very helpful comments. Part of the material used was obtained through CGL2005-03620.

References

Alvarez, F. (1993). Alertness signalling in two rail species. Animal Behaviour, 46, 1229.

Alvarez, F., Braza, F., & Norzagaray, A. (1976). The use of the rump patch in the fallow deer (D. dama). Behaviour, 56, 298.

Alvarez, F., & Sánchez, C. (2003). The features of distraction behaviour and their relationship with physical condition in Rufous Bush Chats. Ethology Ecology & Evolution, 4, 355.

Alvarez, F., Sánchez, C., & Angulo, S. (2006). Tail-flicking and body condition in moorhens Gallinula chloropus. Journal of Field Ornithology, 77, 1.

Bierman, D. J. (2000). Anomalous baseline effects in mainstream emotion research using psychophysiological variables. Proceedings of the 43rd Annual Convention of the Parapsychological Association, 34. [Convention in Freiburg, Germany.]

Bierman, D. J. (2008). Consciousness induced restoration of time-symmetry (CIRTS), a psychophysical theoretical perspective. Proceedings of the 51st Annual Convention of the Parapsychological Association. [Convention in Winchester, UK.]

Bierman, D. J., & Radin, D. I. (1997). Anomalous anticipatory response on randomized future conditions. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 84, 689.

Bierman, D. J., & Radin, D. I. (1999). Anomalous unconscious emotional responses: Evidence for a reversal of the arrow of time. In: S. R. Hameroff, A.W. Kaszniak, & D. J. Chalmers (Eds.). Towards a Science of Consciousness III: The Third Tucson Discussions and Debates (pp. 367–386). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Bierman, D. J., & Scholte, H. S. (2002). Anomalous anticipatory brain activation preceding exposure of emotional and neutral pictures. Proceedings of the 45th Annual Convention of the Parapsychological Association, 25.

Cuthill, I. C., Hart, N. S., Partridge, J. C., Bennett, A. T. D., Hunt, S., & Church, S. C. (2000). Avian colour vision and avian video playback experiments. Acta Ethologica, 3, 29.

Dawkins, M. S., & Woodington, A. (1997). Distance and the presentation of visual stimuli to birds. Animal Behaviour, 54, 1019.

D’Eath, R. B. (1998). Can video images imitate real stimuli in animal behaviour experiments? Biological reviews, 73, 267.

Fleishman, L. J., McClintock, W. J., D’Eath, R. B., Brainard, D. H., & Endler, J. A. (1998). Colour perception and the use of video playback experiments in animal behaviour. Animal Behaviour, 56, 1035.

Galloch, Z., & Bischof, H. J. (2006). Zebra Finches actively choose between live images of conspecifics. Ornithological Science, 5, 57.

Galloch, Z., & Bischof, H. J. (2007). Behavioural responses to video playbacks by zebra finch males. Behavioural Processes, 74, 21.

Globisch, J., Hamm, A. O., Esteves, F., & Öhman, A. (1999). Fear appears fast: Temporal course of startle reflex potentiation in animal fearful subjects. Psychophysiology, 36, 66.

Goodwin, D. (1982). Estrildid Finches of the World. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press

Griffiths, R., Double, M. C., Orr, K., & Dawson, R. J. G. (1998). A DNA test to sex most birds. Molecular Ecology, 7, 1071.

Ikebuchi, M., & Okanoya, K. (1999). Male zebra finches and Bengalese finches emit directed songs to the video images of conspecific females projected onto a TFT display. Zoological Science, 16, 63.

Laszlo, E. (2009). In defense of intuition: Exploring the physical foundations of spontaneous apprehension. Journal of Scientific Exploration, 23, 51.

Lombardi, C. M., & Curio, E. (1985a). Influence of environment on mobbing by zebra finches. Bird Behaviour, 6, 28.

Lombardi, C. M., & Curio, E. (1985b). Social facilitation of mobbing in the zebra finch Taeniopygia guttata. Bird Behaviour, 6, 34.

Marler, P. (2004). Bird calls. Their potential for behavioral neurobiology. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci., 1016, 31.

May, E. C., Paulinyi, T., & Vassy, Z. (2005). Anomalous anticipatory skin conductance response to acoustic stimuli: Experimental results and speculations about a mechanism. The Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine, 11, 695.

McCraty, R., Atkinson, M., & Bradley, R. T. (2004a). Electrophysiological evidence of intuition: Part 1. The surprising role of the heart. The Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine, 10, 133.

McCraty, R., Atkinson, M., & Bradley, R. T. (2004b). Electrophysiological evidence of intuition: Part 2. A system-wide process? The Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine, 10, 325.

Radin, D. I. (1997a). Unconscious perception of future emotions: An experiment in presentiment. Journal of Scientific Exploration, 11, 163.

Radin, D. I. (1997b). The Conscious Universe. San Francisco: HarperEdge

Radin, D. I. (2002). A dog that seems to know when his owner is coming home: Effect of environmental variables. Journal of Scientific Exploration, 16, 579.

Radin, D. I. (2004). Electrodermal presentiments of future emotions. Journal of Scientific Exploration, 18, 253.

Rhine, J. B., Smith, B. M., & Woodruff, J. L. (1938). Experiments bearing on the precognition hypothesis: II. The role of ESP in the shuffling of cards. Journal of Parapsychology, 2, 119.

Sartori, L., Massaccesi, S., Martinelli, M., & Tressoldi, P. (2004). Physiological correlates of ESP: Heart rate differences between targets and nontargets. Journal of Parapsychology, 68, 351.

Schmidt, H. (1969). Quantum processes predicted? New Scientist, October 16, 114.

Schmidt, H. (1993). Non-causality as the earmark of Psi. Journal of Scientific Exploration, 7, 125.

Sheldrake, R., & Smart, P. (1998). A dog that seems to know when its owner is returning: Preliminary investigations. Journal of the Society for Psychical Research, 62, 220.

Sheldrake, R., & Smart, P. (2000). A dog that seems to know when its owner is returning: Videotaped experiments and observations. Journal of Scientific Exploration, 14, 233.

Siegel, S., & Castellan, N. J. (1988). Nonparametric Statistics for the Behavioral Sciences. New York: McGraw-Hill.

Sokal, R. R., & Rohlf, F. J. (1995). Biometry. New York: Freeman.

Spottiswoode, S. J. P., & May, E. C. (2003). Skin conductance prestimulus response: Analyses, artifacts and a pilot study. Journal of Scientific Exploration, 17, 617.

Tressoldi, P. E., Martinelli, M., Massaccesi, S., & Sartori, L. (2005). Heart rate differences between targets and nontargets in intuitive tasks. Human Physiology, 31, 646.

Vassy, Z. (1978). Method of measuring the probability of 1-bit extrasensory information transfer between living organisms. Journal of Parapsychology, 43, 158.

Vermeij, G. J. (1987). Evolution and Escalation: An Ecological History of Life. Princeton, NJ: Princeton Univ. Press.

Vriends, M. M., & Heming-Vriends, T. (2002). Hancock Encyclopedia of Estrildid Finches. Blaine, WA: Hancock House.

Watanabe, S., & Jian, T. (1993). Visual and auditory cues in conspecific discrimination learning in Bengalese finches. Journal of Ethology, 11, 111.

Wildey, C. R. (2001). Biological Response to Stimulus. [M.S. Thesis]. University of Texas, Arlington.

Zann, R. A. (1996). The Zebra Finch. A Synthesis of Field and Laboratory Studies. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Zar, K. H. (1996). Biostatistical Analysis. New Jersey: Prentice Hall.